Discussion
Positive relationships with parents and other caregivers in childhood are related to stronger attachments,39 40 and help develop the ability to trust others.41 When children have been hurt or neglected by caregivers, their ability to trust others may be diminished along with their ability to form attachment bonds,42–44 even subsequently with their own children.45 Moreover, maltreated children can be more likely to interpret neutral facial expressions as angry or aggressive,46 with immediate implications for developing attachment and trust.43 While such limited findings illustrate the impacts ACEs may have on an individual’s ability to trust others, less empirical information is available on how ACEs may affect trust in information and advice from professionals, and public and private organisations. In a largely separate literature, trust in organisations often considers issues such as their consistency, reliability, transparency, ethics and integrity, and proven competence.47–50 However, such work has generally not considered how individuals’ experiences of ACEs may affect their ability to trust information and advice from organisations and professionals for health and other purposes.
Here, our results identified strong associations between exposure to ACEs and greater prevalence of low trust in different support systems, as well as health and other advice and information from professionals, agencies and information sources. Of particular concern to healthcare and public health systems, individuals with ≥4 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs) were between two and three times more likely to have low trust in GPs, hospital doctors, nurses and pharmacists (table 2). Critically, those with a high ACEs count are more likely to have greater healthcare, health protection and health promotion needs.6 Consequently, low trust may result in poorer uptake of health improving behaviour advice, specifically in those most likely to develop non-communicable diseases, infections and other physical and mental health issues at earlier ages. Furthermore, trust in sources of advice is an important predictor of compliance with such advice.51 Consequently, individuals with higher ACE counts may also be less likely to adopt and maintain fidelity to medication regimens, other treatment plans or vaccination schedules.26 52 Health systems should recognise that those most in need of their support may also be least likely to trust their advice. Recent developments in trauma informed approaches to health and well being have begun considering how all systems may better meet the needs of those who have experienced trauma, including ACEs.53 54 However, further empirical work is urgently needed, especially when exposure to ACEs remains a common experience (here, 43.4% had at least one ACE and 10.9% ≥4 ACEs).
Overall, low trust was much more prevalent in advice and information from virtual (online and apps) or remote (NHS 111) health advice sources than directly from health professionals (table 1), although response levels varied by source (see limitations). Sources carrying the NHS brand or associated with health professions were more trusted than their more generic counterparts (eg, low trust in NHS websites vs general internet sites, table 1), with social media the least trusted source (71.5% low trust; table 1). Regardless, there were strong positive associations between multiple ACE exposure and low trust for all such sources (tables 2–4). With increasing proportions of public health advice and service contact moving to remote platforms (eg, phone and internet), lower trust in these services should be of concern. Moreover, individuals with higher ACE counts are often greater users of health systems and, for instance, adjusted prevalence of low trust in health advice from the NHS 111 service rose from 11.9% with 0 ACEs to 24.1% with ≥4 ACEs (figure 1).
Similar increases in low trust with higher ACEs were seen for all services, including health services (tables 1 and 3). The greatest increase in the likelihood of low trust with ACEs was seen for police (≥4 ACEs vs 0 ACEs; aOR 3.81), although adjusted odds also at least doubled in those with ≥4 ACEs for health and social services, charities and government (table 3). While evidence supports individuals with higher ACEs having more experiences as perpetrators and victims of crime,7 55 results here identified an estimated increase in prevalence of low trust in police services from 17.0% (0 ACEs) to 43.8% (≥4 ACEs; figure 1). Some police forces are already considering how they make services more trauma informed in order to better engage and build trust with those with a history of adversity.56 57 Typically, however, in the UK and elsewhere, such measures are not yet an integrated feature of police training and operations.
Across all services and systems, the highest prevalence of reported low trust was seen for government (table 1). However, public health advice is frequently referred to as government guidance or guidelines and issued by government departments. Examples include advice for healthy eating58 and physical activity,59 while in some countries health warning labels on alcohol products are identified as governmental rather than health service or medical advice (eg, USA;60). Our results suggested labelling information and advice as governmental may lead to lower levels of trust in it and potentially provide commercial organisations wishing to lessen its uptake an opportunity to exploit a less trusted title (eg, governmental guidance). The differences in trust for those with high levels of ACEs were particularly stark. An estimated 23.0% of those with ≥4 ACEs reported low trust in health services, rising to 73.7% with low trust in government (figure 1). Consequently, our results suggest that provision of national health related or other guidance should consider the benefits of an exclusive health service brand rather than a governmental one.
Finally, our results suggested very low levels of trust in social media, with considerably greater trust in more traditional information platforms, such as TV/radio programmes (table 1). Again, for all communication platforms, low trust was significantly increased in those with higher ACEs (table 4, figure 1). However, increasing amounts of public funding are being invested in social media messaging in order to reach different population groups. The value of such investments may need recalibrating if three quarters of those with high ACE counts have low trust in social media and even among those with no ACE exposure most individuals have low trust in advice and information on such platforms.
Limitations
The study relied on recall of ACEs by adults, which may be incomplete or inaccurate and, in some cases, participants may have chosen not to disclose certain ACEs despite the reassurance of confidentiality and anonymity. Moreover, an average interview time of 22 min may have impacted some respondents’ focus and compliance over the course of their interview. However, ACE prevalence was comparable with other studies undertaken in the UK.35 While compliance levels were also generally consistent with other ACE surveys (49%35) we cannot identify how ACEs and levels of trust in those choosing not to complete the survey may have affected the results. Here, respondents were able to say if they could not rate trust in any particular individual, agency or service (table 1; online supplemental tables A3–A6) as not all respondents may feel adequately exposed to or informed on each source. The majority of respondents (>90%) could rate each type of individual health professional (ie, GPs, hospital doctors, nurses and pharmacists) and there were no significant relationships between ACE counts and providing such trust ratings (online supplemental table A4). For some information/advice sources and services, ACE count was related to ability to provide a trust rating. Thus social services and NHS 111 were more likely to be given a rating by individuals with higher ACEs (online supplemental tables A4 and A5), which may reflect a greater likelihood of being in contact with such services. Individuals with 2–3 ACEs (vs 0 ACEs) were more likely to rate health apps, general internet sites, virtual health professionals and social media but less likely to rate health services in general (online supplemental tables A4–A6). It is not possible to assess whether this reflects a pattern of accessing remote services more among this group. However, results may better reflect those directly or indirectly exposed to each source rather than any overall population wide prevalence.
We used a range of ACEs typically identified in ACE research tools used by the World Health Organization and US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. However, there is continuing debate about whether other childhood adversities (eg, peer victimisation, community violence) should be included in ACE measurements61–63 and how this would affect relationships with trust requires further studies. Moreover, our results did not measure the severity of ACEs, length of exposure, frequency and age of occurrence,61 all of which may be important in the development of trust. With little work having already been undertaken on the impacts of adversity on trust in the range of individuals and organisations we examined, there were no validated instruments, and the questions used here require further validation and refinement. Our questions did not examine if levels of trust might vary depending on the type of information or service provided but this is an important consideration for further studies. We chose to dichotomise participants trust ratings into low trust (yes 0–4, or no 5–10 on the scale) with the same single cut-off applied to all items. This reflected the aim of the study to examine factors relating specifically to low trust. However, further studies could examine other categorisations including, for instance, very high or very low trust (eg, 8–10 or 0–2 on the 0–10 scale, respectively). Finally, the survey was carried out after a pandemic and during a cost of living crisis,64 and we were not able to ascertain whether trust in information and advice from different sources was affected by these global events and whether any such affects are sustained.