Discussion
In this study of home care recipients receiving primary care from family physicians in Ontario, Canada, we found that the proportion of ED visits, hospitalisations and death during the 1-year study period was lowest for those who spoke a language other than English or French (ie, allophones). These findings are consistent with those of previous studies, which have found that allophones living in minority linguistic communities are less frequent users of hospital-based healthcare services.38 39 This has generally been attributed to barriers to accessing care7 40 41 and to cultural differences resulting in a preference for community-based care rather than hospital-based care.42 43 To our knowledge, only one study has compared mortality rates across all linguistic groups in the Canadian population; this study found that allophones living in Quebec had significantly lower all-cause mortality than both anglophones and francophones.44 Differential mortality among allophones may also partly be explained by the healthy immigrant effect, which postulates that individuals who have immigrated to Canada have more positive health status on arrival when compared with age-matched and sex-matched Canadian-born citizens due to lifestyle behaviours and self-selection related to immigration policies and procedures.45 The advantage in health status seen among Canadian immigrants has been shown to dissipate after 2–10 years45; however, differential mortality persists, with one study reporting lower age-standardised mortality rates at 20 years after immigration to Canada.46 Only 1 in 10 allophones in our study were identified as immigrants; however, our study was limited by the fact that we were only able to identify recent immigrants (ie, those who immigrated since 1985), which likely resulted in misclassification of immigration status for older home care recipients who may have immigrated before 1985 (although there is expected waning of healthy immigrant effect in these individuals).
Allophones who received language-concordant primary care were less likely to use hospital-based healthcare services and to experience death when compared with allophones who received language-discordant primary care. We hypothesise that these results could be due, at least in part, to language barriers contributing to lower levels of satisfaction with their family physician as well as poorer quality of care received.1 7 40 41 Clear and effective patient–physician communication is associated with enhanced patient cooperation and engagement,47 which in turn has the potential to improve patient outcomes. These findings are supported by American studies which have shown that Spanish-speaking patients with asthma who received care from Spanish-speaking physicians were less likely to miss appointments, omit medications or visit the ED.11 Furthermore, Spanish-speaking patients with diabetes who receive language-discordant primary care had worse glycaemic control12 and low-density-lipoprotein control,13 as well as poorer participation in diabetic foot care programmes.48 Another study conducted in Ontario, Canada showed that patients with TB who received care from respirologists who spoke their primary language were less likely to die within 1 year.14 Poor patient–physician communication can affect the ability to obtain accurate information to be able to make timely diagnoses and formulate management plans,1 3 41 which in turn could result in patients seeking hospital-based healthcare services to accelerate their diagnostic workup or to address unmet needs (including lack of information due to communication barrier).
Interestingly, outcomes for francophones were not statistically impacted by the language of their family physician. This finding can likely be attributed to the relatively higher rate of bilingualism among francophones when compared with allophones. According to the 2021 Census, the proportion of francophones and allophones (defined by language spoken most often at home) over the age of 65 in Ontario, Canada who are able to conduct a conversation in English is 86.6% and 68.7%, respectively.49 Furthermore, a previous study of home care recipients in Ontario, Canada found that more than half of allophones had low English proficiency, while less than 10% of francophones were deemed to have low English proficiency.50 Therefore, relatively fewer francophones will experience language barriers when interacting with their family physician (all of whom should be able to speak English since this is a requirement to practice medicine in Ontario), and the language barriers experienced by francophones are likely to be less severe when compared with those experienced by allophones.3 Finally, while French is not recognised as an official language at the provincial level, the French Language Services Act identifies a number of healthcare facilities (both community based and hospital based) which are required by law to provide services in French in regions of the province with the highest proportion of francophone residents (defined as geographical regions where francophones make up at least 10% of the population).21 This includes, but is not limited to, public health programmes providing support for addictions, dementia, disability, mental health, preventative care and sexually transmitted infections.21 As a result, francophones may have been more likely to access language-concordant community-based resources rather than use hospital-based healthcare services.
The findings of this study contribute to the paucity of research on the topic of patient–provider language concordance/discordance in Canada. We previously showed that home care recipients admitted to a hospital in Ontario who received more than half of their care from physicians who shared a mutually intelligible language had lower risks of adverse events, shorter stays in hospital and lower risks of in-hospital death.16 By considering patients in two different settings (ie, inpatient vs outpatient), this study provides additional information regarding the way in which linguistically concordant/discordant care is offered to patients in Ontario. First, this study determined language concordance by using the language(s) of the patient’s primary care provider while our prior study determined language concordance by calculating a weighted average of the languages spoken by all physicians who provided care during the hospitalisations.16 Next, the outcomes under study were different, with an emphasis on hospital-based healthcare utilisation (ED visits, hospitalisations) in this study, versus in-hospital outcomes (harm and length of stay) in our prior study.16 While less than half (43.9%) of allophones received language-concordant care in this study (outpatient setting), only 1.6% of allophones received language-concordant care in our prior study (inpatient setting).16 A similar trend was observed for francophones, who were more likely to receive language-concordant care as outpatients (61.3%) rather than as inpatients (44.4%).16 Our prior study also identified much larger effect sizes for inpatients when compared with the effect sizes identified for outpatients in this study,16 which may be explained by access to and availability of language-concordant care (greater in the outpatient setting compared with the inpatient setting, where patients may not have the opportunity to seek care from a physician who speaks their primary language).
Limitations
We obtained patient language from home care assessments, during which interviewers recorded the patient’s primary language.24 Since interviewers can only record one language for each home care recipient, we may have overestimated the prevalence of language discordance among those who speak multiple languages (including English). However, such misclassification would result in a bias towards the null, as patients would be incorrectly identified as having received language-discordant care (higher risk of poor outcomes) when they in fact received language-concordant care (lower risk of poor outcomes). Furthermore, it is possible that interviewers may have recorded home care recipient’s language as English to avoid the need to obtain interpreter services. This would result in francophones and allophones being incorrectly identified as Anglophones, which would also minimise the disparities experienced by francophones and allophones when compared with anglophones. Next, we do not know whether francophone and allophone patients identified as receiving language-concordant care did in fact communicate with their physician in their primary language or, in the case of allophones included in a group with multiple languages, whether the communication occurred in a language that was mutually intelligible with their primary language. As above, misclassification of allophone patients as having received language-concordant care when they in fact received language-discordant care (eg, because they experienced a language barrier when interacting with a physician who had proficiency in a language that was mutually intelligible but not identical to their primary language) should bias the results towards the null. We also do not know whether interpretation services were used for patients identified as receiving language-discordant care. It is likely that some francophone and allophone patients had family members, friends or other healthcare providers who provided interpretation during their interactions with their family physician. Unfortunately, professional interpreters are not systematically employed across Canadian healthcare facilities,22 and studies have shown that non-professional interpreters (ie, family members, friends and hospital employees) do not consistently improve outcomes. Therefore, we feel that interpreter use is unlikely to have had a significant impact on the outcomes reported in our study.51 52 Finally, physician language obtained from the CPSO has not yet been validated and, unfortunately, was missing for almost half of physicians in our study. However, since physician language is recorded prior to and independently from patient outcomes, any misclassification should be non-differential, thereby leading the results to be biased towards the null.